journal
MENU ▼
Read by QxMD icon Read
search

I-Perception

journal
https://www.readbyqxmd.com/read/29348911/reduction-of-flicker-in-four-stroke-motion-of-color-images
#1
Takahiro Kawabe, Shin'ya Nishida
When two sequential video frames extracted from a single video clip are followed by the negative of the two frames, a viewer often experiences a visual illusion whereby a scene in the frames continuously moves in a single direction (four-stroke apparent motion). To create a four-stroke apparent motion display, the image intensities of the whole of the second pair of images are reversed. However, this intensity reversal creates a strong impression of flicker that can be undesirable for comfortable viewing. This study reports that four-stroke apparent motion can be induced by only reversing the luminance intensities in those spatial areas which contain motion signals in high-pass filtered images...
January 2018: I-Perception
https://www.readbyqxmd.com/read/29344333/inverting-the-facing-the-viewer-bias-for-biological-motion-stimuli
#2
Séamas Weech, Nikolaus F Troje
Depth-ambiguous point-light walkers are most frequently seen as facing-the-viewer (FTV). It has been argued that the FTV bias depends on recognising the stimulus as a person. Accordingly, reducing the social relevance of biological motion by presenting stimuli upside down has been shown to reduce FTV bias. Here, we replicated the experiment that reported this finding and added stick figure walkers to the task in order to assess the effect of explicit shape information on facing bias for inverted figures. We measured the FTV bias for upright and inverted stick figure walkers and point-light walkers presented in different azimuth orientations...
January 2018: I-Perception
https://www.readbyqxmd.com/read/29344332/kitaoka-s-tomato-two-simple-explanations-based-on-information-in-the-stimulus
#3
Arthur Shapiro, Laysa Hedjar, Erica Dixon, Akiyoshi Kitaoka
Kitaoka's Tomato is a color illusion in which a semitransparent blue-green field is placed on top of a red object (a tomato). The tomato appears red even though the pixels would appear green if viewed in isolation. We show that this phenomenon can be explained by a high-pass filter and by histogram equalization. The results suggest that this illusion does not require complex inferences about color constancy; rather, the tomato's red is available in the physical stimulus at the appropriate spatial scale and dynamic range...
January 2018: I-Perception
https://www.readbyqxmd.com/read/29348910/determinants-of-colour-constancy-and-the-blue-bias
#4
David Weiss, Christoph Witzel, Karl Gegenfurtner
We investigated several sensory and cognitive determinants of colour constancy across 40 illumination hues. In the first experiment, we measured colour naming for the illumination and for the colour induced by the illumination on the colorimetric grey. Results confirmed that the induced colours are approximately complementary to the colour of the illumination. In the second experiment, we measured colour constancy using achromatic adjustments. Average colour constancy was perfect under the blue daylight illumination and decreased in colour directions away from the blue daylight illumination due to undershooting and a strong blue bias...
November 2017: I-Perception
https://www.readbyqxmd.com/read/29344331/the-peripheral-flicker-illusion
#5
Hiroyuki Ito, Tomomi Koizumi
A new illusion is reported. A visual object suddenly appearing on a red background sometimes causes an impression of flicker or double flash. In Experiment 1, a red, green, or blue object was presented on a red, green, blue, or gray background. Participants evaluated the illusion strength in reference to the physical flicker of a gray object presented in central vision. The results show that the green or blue object presented on the red background caused the illusion. In Experiment 2, the effect of retinal eccentricity on the illusion was tested...
November 2017: I-Perception
https://www.readbyqxmd.com/read/29317999/volume-completion-between-contour-fragments-at-discrete-depths
#6
Peter Ulric Tse
Building on the modal and amodal completion work of Kanizsa, Carman and Welch showed that binocular stereo viewing of two disparate images can give rise to a percept of 3D curved, nonclosed illusory contours and surfaces. Here, it is shown that binocular presentation can also give rise to the percept of closed curved surfaces or volumes that appear to vary smoothly across discrete depths in binocularly fused images, although in fact only two binocular disparities are discretely defined between corresponding contour elements of the inducing elements...
November 2017: I-Perception
https://www.readbyqxmd.com/read/29309062/erratum
#7
(no author information available yet)
[This corrects the article DOI: 10.1177/2041669517743523.].
November 2017: I-Perception
https://www.readbyqxmd.com/read/29308179/seeing-colors-cultural-and-environmental-influences-on-episodic-memory
#8
Kimele Persaud, Pernille Hemmer, Celeste Kidd, Steven Piantadosi
Expectations learned from our perceptual experiences, culture, and language can shape how we perceive, interact with, and remember features of the past. Here, we questioned whether environment also plays a role. We tested recognition memory for color in Bolivia's indigenous Tsimanè people, who experience a different color environment than standard U.S. POPULATIONS: We found that memory regressed differently between the groups, lending credence to the idea that environmental variations engender differences in expectations, and in turn perceptual memory for color...
November 2017: I-Perception
https://www.readbyqxmd.com/read/29308178/influence-of-visual-prism-adaptation-on-auditory-space-representation
#9
Klaudia Pochopien, Manfred Fahle
Prisms shifting the visual input sideways produce a mismatch between the visual versus felt position of one's hand. Prism adaptation eliminates this mismatch, realigning hand proprioception with visual input. Whether this realignment concerns exclusively the visuo-(hand)motor system or it generalizes to acoustic inputs is controversial. We here show that there is indeed a slight influence of visual adaptation on the perceived direction of acoustic sources. However, this shift in perceived auditory direction can be fully explained by a subconscious head rotation during prism exposure and by changes in arm proprioception...
November 2017: I-Perception
https://www.readbyqxmd.com/read/29308177/action-recognition-in-a-crowded-environment
#10
Laura Fademrecht, Judith Nieuwenhuis, Isabelle Bülthoff, Nick Barraclough, Stephan de la Rosa
So far, action recognition has been mainly examined with small point-light human stimuli presented alone within a narrow central area of the observer's visual field. Yet, we need to recognize the actions of life-size humans viewed alone or surrounded by bystanders, whether they are seen in central or peripheral vision. Here, we examined the mechanisms in central vision and far periphery (40° eccentricity) involved in the recognition of the actions of a life-size actor (target) and their sensitivity to the presence of a crowd surrounding the target...
November 2017: I-Perception
https://www.readbyqxmd.com/read/29270286/the-inference-of-friendly-communicative-atmosphere-created-by-geometric-shapes
#11
Masahide Yuasa
Many previous studies on inference of social behaviors using geometric shapes have explored causality, animacy, intention, and desire inferred from the movements of such shapes; however, inference of communicative atmosphere in terms of friendliness/antagonism using geometric shapes has not yet been studied well. This study investigated how a friendly/antagonistic communicative atmosphere was inferred from the movement of two egg shapes. We developed animations for these shapes involving forward/backward/parallel tilts with coincidence/incoincidence of synchronous movement...
November 2017: I-Perception
https://www.readbyqxmd.com/read/29270285/dynamic-volume-completion-and-deformation
#12
Peter Ulric Tse
A new class of dynamic volume completion is introduced, where image elements (e.g., occluding semi-ellipses placed at the edge of an object) can link across a gap between two or more objects, leading to the perception of illusory volumes that deform as those image elements are set into relative motion. These new demonstrations provide further evidence that volume completion is not dictated solely by contour relatability constraints, but is instead a dynamic process of 3D shape construction that also takes into account dynamic cues to object shape, even in the absence of any contour relatability whatsoever...
November 2017: I-Perception
https://www.readbyqxmd.com/read/29250308/increasing-the-complexity-of-the-illumination-may-reduce-gloss-constancy
#13
Gunnar Wendt, Franz Faul
We examined in which way gradual changes in the geometric structure of the illumination affect the perceived glossiness of a surface. The test stimuli were computer-generated three-dimensional scenes with a single test object that was illuminated by three point light sources, whose relative positions in space were systematically varied. In the first experiment, the subjects were asked to adjust the microscale smoothness of a match object illuminated by a single light source such that it has the same perceived glossiness as the test stimulus...
November 2017: I-Perception
https://www.readbyqxmd.com/read/29238513/color-constancy-in-two-dimensional-and-three-dimensional-scenes-effects-of-viewing-methods-and-surface-texture
#14
Takuma Morimoto, Yoko Mizokami, Hirohisa Yaguchi, Steven L Buck
There has been debate about how and why color constancy may be better in three-dimensional (3-D) scenes than in two-dimensional (2-D) scenes. Although some studies have shown better color constancy for 3-D conditions, the role of specific cues remains unclear. In this study, we compared color constancy for a 3-D miniature room (a real scene consisting of actual objects) and 2-D still images of that room presented on a monitor using three viewing methods: binocular viewing, monocular viewing, and head movement...
November 2017: I-Perception
https://www.readbyqxmd.com/read/29230277/the-sense-of-being-watched-is-modulated-by-arousal-and-duration-of-the-perceptual-episode
#15
Vera M Hesslinger, Claus-Christian Carbon, Heiko Hecht
The mere presence of a depiction of eyes can elicit a sense of being watched in the perceiver. To this date, the factors affecting the intensity of this sense of being watched, however, have not been investigated. In the present experiment, we tested the impact of two potentially relevant variables: arousal (manipulated using specific musical pieces) and duration of the perceptual episode (manipulated using presentation times of 200 ms and 10 s, respectively). We asked participants to report how intensely they felt being watched while we exposed them to various observation cues ranging from human eyes to surveillance cameras...
November 2017: I-Perception
https://www.readbyqxmd.com/read/29230276/seasonal-changes-in-color-preferences-are-linked-to-variations-in-environmental-colors-a-longitudinal-study-of-fall
#16
Karen B Schloss, Isobel A Heck
People form associations between colors and entities, which influence their evaluations of the world. These evaluations are dynamic, as specific associations become more or less active in people's minds over time. We investigated how evaluations of colors (color preferences) changed over the course of fall, as color-associated fall entities became more prevalent in the environment. Participants judged their preferences for the same set of colors during nine testing sessions over 11 weeks during fall. We categorized the colors as Leaf and Non-Leaf Colors by matching them to leaves collected during the same period...
November 2017: I-Perception
https://www.readbyqxmd.com/read/29225768/about-face-matching-unfamiliar-faces-across-rotations-of-view-and-lighting
#17
Simone Favelle, Harold Hill, Peter Claes
Matching the identities of unfamiliar faces is heavily influenced by variations in their images. Changes to viewpoint and lighting direction during face perception are commonplace across yaw and pitch axes and can result in dramatic image differences. We report two experiments that, for the first time, factorially investigate the combined effects of lighting and view angle on matching performance for unfamiliar faces. The use of three-dimensional head models allowed control of both lighting and viewpoint. We found viewpoint effects in the yaw axis with little to no effect of lighting...
November 2017: I-Perception
https://www.readbyqxmd.com/read/29225767/pink-cricket-balls-through-rose-tinted-glasses-enhancing-interceptive-timing
#18
Joshua M Adie, Derek H Arnold
Cricket is a popular but potentially dangerous sport. It is played with a hard ball that can travel at great speeds. Serious injuries, including fatalities, have occurred when balls have struck participants. The game is traditionally played during daylight with a dark red ball, but recent games have been played during the day and at night using a 'pink' ball. We have reported data that seemed to justify concerns raised regarding the visibility of these new pink balls, as they were revealed to have a very low luminance contrast against pertinent backgrounds during twilight...
November 2017: I-Perception
https://www.readbyqxmd.com/read/29225766/on-the-discovery-of-monocular-rivalry-by-tscherning-in-1898-translation-and-review
#19
Robert P O'Shea, Urte Roeber, Nicholas J Wade
Monocular rivalry was named by Breese in 1899. He made prolonged observation of superimposed orthogonal gratings; they fluctuated in clarity with either one or the other grating occasionally being visible alone. A year earlier, Tscherning observed similar fluctuations with a grid of vertical and horizontal lines and with other stimuli; we draw attention to his prior account. Monocular rivalry has since been shown to occur with a wide variety of superimposed patterns with several independent rediscoveries of it...
November 2017: I-Perception
https://www.readbyqxmd.com/read/29225765/perspective-space-as-a-model-for-distance-and-size-perception
#20
Casper J Erkelens
In the literature, perspective space has been introduced as a model of visual space. Perspective space is grounded on the perspective nature of visual space during both binocular and monocular vision. A single parameter, that is, the distance of the vanishing point, transforms the geometry of physical space into that of perspective space. The perspective-space model predicts perceived angles, distances, and sizes. The model is compared with other models for distance and size perception. Perspective space predicts that perceived distance and size as a function of physical distance are described by hyperbolic functions...
November 2017: I-Perception
journal
journal
43849
1
2
Fetch more papers »
Fetching more papers... Fetching...
Read by QxMD. Sign in or create an account to discover new knowledge that matter to you.
Remove bar
Read by QxMD icon Read
×

Search Tips

Use Boolean operators: AND/OR

diabetic AND foot
diabetes OR diabetic

Exclude a word using the 'minus' sign

Virchow -triad

Use Parentheses

water AND (cup OR glass)

Add an asterisk (*) at end of a word to include word stems

Neuro* will search for Neurology, Neuroscientist, Neurological, and so on

Use quotes to search for an exact phrase

"primary prevention of cancer"
(heart or cardiac or cardio*) AND arrest -"American Heart Association"